Mangile’s Pigeon Pages
Taken from the Chicago Tribune
October 7, 1900 - page 38
LIVES AMONG 500 PIGEONS.
Important Experiments by Prof.
Whitman of University of Chicago.
With 500 birds as his companions and subjects
Professor Charles O. Whitman of the University of Chicago has for six
years been studying and conducting experiments which before long it is
promised will give results of the utmost scientific value and which
will pass even for the layman far beyond the bounds of the merely
interesting.
Every person who passes by the handsome stone residence, 223 East
Fifth-fourth street, and who lets his eyes rise above the level of the
steps, is struck by the sight of scores of pigeons brushing their
burnished feathers against the glass of the bay windows which form the
outer side of the great cages in which they are confined. The
residence is the home of Professor Whitman, who is making the pigeon
tribe a study, and who, in order to do the work more thoroughly,
literally lives with his subjects.
Professor Whitman has not only made home companions of half a thousand
wild and domestic doves, but, in order that he might not have his
studies interrupted, they have been his traveling companions as
well. Last week, in the house of the student and in the grounds
at the rear, there was enacted a scene of the most intense interest to
all scientists who have made animal intelligence a study. Cages
containing the 500 pigeons had their doors opened wide and the birds
set at liberty. They have just been brought back from the three
months stay at Wood’s Hole, Mass., where Professor Whitman had pursued
his summer bird studies. Upon the opening of the door of its cage
each pigeon unerringly made its way instantly to the particular cote
which it had occupied before leaving Chicago for its summer outing by
the sea. Nine months hence, when the birds are taken back to
Wood’s Hole they will be turned loose again and each will fly at once
to the cage where it passed the summer which has just waned to autumn.
Studying Laws of Heredity.
Professor Charles O. Whitman is a believer in the theory that the study
of the habits, instincts, and intelligence of the lower animals has a
fundamental relation to the study of man’s menial development. He
is devoting himself closely to minute investigation of the laws of
heredity and all the correlated phenomena that springs
there-from. This quiet university scholar who literally has
pigeons at his bed board and books has succeeded in his South Side home
in securing some of the most remarkable results from cross-breeding
experiments known to the pigeon fancying world. He has in his
collection, or, perhaps as he would prefer it, among his companions,
representatives of perhaps every know kind of pigeon that the world
produces. Further this he has with the sole exception of a few
birds owned in Milwaukee, the only known living specimens of the
American passenger, or wild-pigeon, now extinct in the wild state, but
which only a comparatively few years ago was the most widely
distributed and numerous of American birds.
All Kinds of Pigeons.
Within the scope of the collection Professor Whitman has pigeons which
in size are no larger than a sparrow, and others, the crowned pigeons
of Australia, whose bulk is as great as that of the bald eagle.
There are in some of the cages which hide the walls of the student’s
library featherless little-creatures just out of the shell which claim
for parents mothers and fathers of two totally different pigeon tribes,
and of such extremes in size that the one parent in some instances
would easily make three of the other in weight, length, and breadth of
wing. One curious feature of the collection is the sight of some
hard working ring doves feeding, cuddling, and doing their best to
bring up in the way they should to young ones which have no natural
claim on them whatsoever, before the offspring of some pair in an
adjoining cage who lack either the inclination or the ability to bring
up children properly.
It might be supposed, possibly, that Professor Whitman chose pigeons as
a study because of their superior intelligence. As a matter of
fact the birds are less intelligent than many of their feathered
brothers of other families, and they were chosen for study because of
their adaptability to conditions of confinement, but more particularly
because the great number of varieties in the family gave an unlimited
field for crossing experiments and resulting observations on the
effects of heredity. If a person wishes to use a perfect example
of reversion or atavism Professor Whitman will show him a pigeon of
which every feather is as white as a spring snowdrop, while its parents
in the next cage are the one as black as a nun’s hood and the other as
gray as a foggy dawn. One of the grandparents of this bird was
white and the coloring of a grandchild simply shows reversion to a
former type.
Touching the reason for his keeping in such close contact with the
subjects of his study, Professor Whitman says this: “The life history
of animals from the primordial germ cell to the end of the life cycle;
their daily periodical, and seasonal routines; their habits, instincts,
intelligence, and peculiarities of behavior under varying conditions;
their geographical distribution; their physiological activities
individually and collectively; their variations, adaptations, breeding
and crossing – in short, biology of animals, is beginning to take its
place beside the more strictly morphological studies which have so long
monopolized the attention of naturalists. The essentials to
understanding any peculiar case of animal behavior are almost
invariably overlooked by inexperienced observers, and the best trained
biologist is liable to the same oversight, especially if the habits of
the animal are not familiar. The qualifications absolutely
indispensable to reliable diagnosis of an animal’s conduct is an
intimate acquaintance with the creature’s normal life, its habits and
instincts. Little can be expected in this most important field of
comparative psychology until investigators realize that such
qualification is not furnished by parlor psychology. It means
nothing less than years of close study – the long continued, patient
observation, experiment, and reflection, best exemplified in Darwin’s
work.”
Cause of Tumbling Habit.
Pigeon fanciers and thousands of other people as well, for that matter,
have always been puzzled to account for the origin of the instinct of
tumbling, a habit which a certain breed of the birds indulges in
constantly, and which makes of the tumblers a special prized
class. Through his long study of the pigeons, both caged and at
liberty, Professor Whitman has arrived at a certain conclusions
touching this tumbling practice, which are as interesting as they are
new. He says that the probably source of the origin of tumbling
was undoubtedly a general action instinctively performed by the
ordinary dovecote pigeon.
“I have noticed a great many times,” said Professor Whitman, “that
common pigeons when on the point of being overtaken and seized by a
hawk suddenly flirt themselves directly downward in a manner suggestive
of tumbling and thus elude the hawk’s swoop. The hawk is carried
on by its momentum and often gives up the chase on the first
failure. In one case I saw the chase renewed three times and
eluded with success each time. The pigeon was a white dovecote
bird with a trace of fantail blood. I saw this pigeon repeatedly
pursued by a swift hawk during one winter and it invariably escaped in
the same way. I have seen the same performance in other dovecote
pigeons under similar circumstances.
“But this is not all. It is well known that dovecote pigeons
delight in quite extended daily flights, circling about their home. I
once raised two pairs of these birds by hand in a place several miles
from any other pigeons. Soon after they were able to fly about
they began these flights, usually in the morning. I frequently
saw one or more of the flock while in the middle of a high flight and
sweeping along swiftly, suddenly plunge downward, often zigzaging with
a quick helter-skelter flirting of the wings. The behavior often
looked like play, and probably it was that in most cases. I
incline to think, however, that it was sometimes prompted by some
degree of alarm. In such flights the birds would frequently get
separated and one thus falling behind would hasten its flight to the
utmost speed in order to overtake its companions. Under such
circumstances they stray bird coming from the rear might be mistaken
for a moment for a hawk in pursuit and one or more of the birds about
to be overtaken would be thus induced to resort to the tumbling method
of throwing themselves out of reach of danger. The same act is
often performed at the start as the pigeon leaves its stand. The
movement is so quick and crazy in the aimlessness that the bird often
seems to be in danger of dashing against the ground, but it always
clears every object. As this act is performed by young and old
alike, and by young birds that have never learned it by example, it
must be regarded as instinctive and I venture to say that is probably
represents the foundation of the more highly developed tumbling
instinct.”
Pouting Due to Heredity.
The instinct of pouting among pigeons has received a large share of
Professor Whitman’s attention, as it likewise did that of Darwin.
“Darwin,” said Professor Whitman, “saw at once from the nature of the
actions that they could not have been taught, but must have appeared
naturally, to use his own words, through probably afterwards vastly
improved by the continued selection of those birds which showed the
strongest propensity. Darwin then postulates as the foundation of
each instinct a propensity - that is, something given in the
constitution. This view of the matter is quite incompatible with
the habit theory, though in entire account with the theory adopted in
the case of the neuter insects.
“I believe the case is much stronger than Darwin suspected and that the
pouting shows not the genesis of instinct from habit, but from a
preexisting congenital basis. Such a basis of the pouting
instinct exists in every dovecote pigeon and is already an organized
instinct, differing from that displayed in the typical pouter only in
degree. I could show that the same instinct is almost universal
among pigeons. Just let me call attention to the instinct as
exhibited in common pigeons. Look at the male pigeon while cooing
to his mate or his neighbors. He inflates his throat and crop,
and this feature is invariably shown in the act of cooing and often
continues for some moments after the cooing ceases. Compare the
pouter with the common pigeon and notice how he increases the inflation
whenever he begins cooing. The pouter’s behavior is nothing but
the universal instinct enormously exaggerated as any attentive observer
may readily discover for himself under favorable circumstances.”
Explains Alleged Insanity.
Romanes, the scientist, in his “Mental Evolution of Animals,” gave wide
publicity and the weight of his own authority to the story of a pigeon
that went insane. One of the results of Professor Whitman’s
investigation is a refutation of the Insanity theory of Romanes and the
giving to the scientific world of some instances of more ‘insane”
actions on the part of perfectly normal pigeons that those attributed
to the supposedly crazy bird. The pigeon of Romanes was a white
fantail, which became, as the original report went, the victim of a
peculiar infatuation. The story goes: “No eccentricity whatever
was remarked in the pigeon'’ conduct until one day I chanced to pick up
a ginger beer bottle of the ordinary brownstone description. I
flung it into the yard, where it fell immediately below the
pigeon-house. That instant down flew pater famillas, and to
my no small astonishment commenced a series of genuflections, evidently
doing homage to the bottle. He strutted round and round it,
bowing and scraping and cooing and performing the most ludicrous antics
I ever beheld on the part of an enamored pigeon. Nor did he cease these
performances until we removed the bottle; and, which proved that this
singular aberration of instinct had become a fixed delusion, whenever
the bottle was placed or thrown in the yard the same ridiculous scene
was enacted. The pigeon came flying down as quickly as when his
dinner was thrown out, and he continued his antics as long as the
bottle was allowed to remain. Sometimes this would go on for
hours, all the other pigeons treating his movements with the most
contemptuous indifference and taking no notice whatever of the bottle.”
Saw His Image Reflected.
Mr. Romanes believed that this pigeon was affected with a strong and
persistent monomania, and said that, although it was well known that
insanity was not an uncommon thing among animals, this was the only
case he had met with of a conspicuous derangement of the instinctive as
distinguished from the rational faculties. Professor Whitman had
this case in mind during the course of his pigeon studies, and says
plumply that this particular pigeon, whose behavior had given it so
wide a fame as a lunatic, was undoubtedly a perfectly normal
bird. ‘I have seen,” he said, “a white fantail play in the same
way to a shadow on the floor, and when the shadow fell on a crust of
bread he at once adopted the bread as the object of his affection and
went through all the bowings and genuflexions ascribed to the “insane”
bird, even to repeating the behavior every time I place the same piece
of bread on the floor of his pen. The chances are that the
“crazy” bird saw his shadow upon the bottle or his image faintly
reflected on it surface.”
A large number of Professor Whitman’s pigeons are wild birds. The
two crowned pigeons of Australia, the largest of the pigeon tribe, go
fluttering with fear at the approach of any one save him who feeds and
owns them. In a cage just beyond is a naturalized Filipino
pigeon, perhaps the most beautiful of all the many varieties that go to
make up the family. This bird is called the “bleeding heart”
because of the brilliant red splash upon the whiteness of the
breast. At a little distance it looks as though the birds was
wearing a damask rosebud. There are pigeons from China, from
Africa, from South America, and practically from all known lands.
Experiments in Crossing.
In the crossing and recrossing of the different varieties which
Professor Whitman has accomplished he is given the opportunity that he
seeks to see which of the parent’s traits are the more strongly
impressed upon the offspring. The professor has succeeded in
crossing the American wild pigeon with several of the different
relatives. The mourning dove, so common in Illinois utterly
unlike it is appearance and characteristics. Among the most
prized birds in the collection are some blue rocks, captured in the
wild state on the coast of England. These birds of which there
are few left in the sate of freedom, are the counterparts of the hardy
blue rock, which for years has been domesticated and which is used on
account of it strong flight at nearly all trap shooting
contests. The domestic birds are the descendants of the
wild blue rocks captured generations ago.
While Professor Whitman’s residence is today in large part given over
to the occupancy of pigeons, within a few weeks almost all the
available wall space in every room will be lined with cages, for the
cold weather will necessitate the removal of several hundred additional
birds to the warmth of indoor quarters,. As the professor put it:
“Companions to the number of 500 might seem too many for comfort, but
then their variety is infinite and their interest never
lags.”
# # #